home psychotherapy news general knowledge consultant trend education organization
 
 
  Certificate
  Authorization
 

Reducing the gap between research and practice in school psychology

THOMAS J. KEHLE AND MELISSA A. BRAY
University of Connecticut
We argue that the existence of the gap is perhaps a result of an overallegiance to the medical
model and the lack of measurable criteria regarding the definition of an educated and psychologically
healthy student. Further, an additional and equally daunting problem for school psychological
practice is that it is influenced by general education that has scant evidence of
accumulated scientifically based knowledge that demonstratively improves the education of children.
The belief that educational practice continually increases in its effectiveness is simply not
warranted. Consequently, educational practice is in constant reform and subjected to legal mandates
that are most often equally ineffective and at times even detrimental. The intent of this
paper is to suggest ways, within this broader milieu, to reduce the gap between science and
practice. © 2005 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Despite sustained criticism of school psychological practices devoid of scientific bases, many
still enjoy widespread popularity. The allegiance to these practices is at least partially caused by
the practitioner¡¯s overreliance on the medical model, intuition, tradition, and the belief in the
righteousness of legislation. School psychological practice should be a scientifically based problemsolving
inquiry to determine what works best to promote children¡¯s academic and social functioning.
Inquiry is a quest for objectivity that is, however, always associated with some degree of error.
However, there are some popular practices that contain considerable error and should not be
employed, such as the use of projective techniques and profile analyses. The initial task to address
this gap between science and practice is to identify why such practices are employed without
sufficient scientific support, and further why more appropriate practices are not embraced.
The Influence of Legislation
Focusing on one of these issues, legislation that is not firmly based in science promotes and
tends to justify inappropriate practices (e.g., Education of All Handicapped Children¡¯s Act, 1975;
and its subsequent reauthorizations, and the No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, Public Law 107¨C
110). The passage of and compliance with these eventually becomes synonymous with moral and
good practice. Noncompliance or criticism of components of the public law, such as Kehle and
Guidubaldi¡¯s (1980) questioning the worth of team placement and individual educational plans on
enhancing the academic and social competence of students, is often met with scorn and viewed as
something wrong or even immoral (Kehle & Guidubaldi, 1982). After 27 years, there exists, as far
as we are aware, no convincing evidence of the worth of these two components (i.e., team placement
decisions and individual educational plans) with respect to either promoting students¡¯ academic
or social competencies. Nevertheless, to suggest that team placement meetings, or the
design of IEPs not be continued because they are ineffectual, and furthermore are an incredible
waste of an inordinate amount of professional, staff, and parental time, would be considered
nothing less than heretical.
The No Child Left Behind Act of 2001 (NCLB) is based on the assumption that increased
accountability, greater freedom of choice for students and parents, greater flexibility on how states
and local education agencies allocate Federal education monies, and an emphasis on reading,
particularly in the primary grades will substantially improve the effectiveness of educational prac-
Correspondence to: Thomas J. Kehle, University of Connecticut, Department of Educational Psychology, Storrs, CT
06269. E-mail: kehle@uconn.edu
Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 42(5), 2005 © 2005 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/pits.20093
577
tice promoting children¡¯s academic competencies. The accountability component of NCLB is
based on annual testing of all students in grades 3 through 8 and annual statewide assessment of
progress toward meeting state proficiency goals within 12 years. These proficiency goals are based
on the performance of the state¡¯s lowest-achieving demographic group, or on its lowest achieving
schools in reading and mathematics. The academic and professional development program components
of NCLB are based on research and superficially are quite appealing. However there are
several fundamental assumptions of NCLB that not only make the effort fool hearty, but very well
could further erode faith in public education. For example, supposedly all teachers must be ¡°highly
qualified.¡± Perhaps the best definition of ¡°highly qualified¡± teachers is those that evidence annual
academic achievement of their students beyond that what would be normally predicted based on
maturation alone. The determination of a cut point based on some standard deviation units beyond
that normally expected is certainly subjective, and most probably unachievable, and most assuredly
almost impossible to ascertain for each elementary school teacher in grades 3 through 8.
However, anything less than the ¡°academic growth of their students¡± criteria will likely further
attenuate the not too rigid current standards.
The notion that funding would be available to support scientifically based instructional techniques
to teach reading is also somewhat of a myth. What would these be? Are they to be developed
at some future date? We are simply unaware of any available reading program designed for
elementary grade children that results in appreciable and enduring gains in reading comprehension
scores. The assumption that NCLB will afford the opportunity for all children by the third grade
the ability to read is revolutionary in that such has never occurred previously in the history of
American public education. Finally, the fundamental goal of NCLB, that schools will achieve
100% proficiency in reading, mathematics, and science in 12 years, is its most serious flaw. It
represents the denial of the existence of individual differences and the Gaussian curve. Specifi-
cally, with regard to children¡¯s reading comprehension, the focus of NCLB represents more wishful
and socially acceptable thinking rather than sensitivity to evidence, although incontrovertible
but most often dismissed, that indicates the genetic characteristics ¡°that determine individual
differences in IQ are largely the same as those that determine individual differences in reading
skills.¡± (Brody, 1992, p. 258). Cardon, DiLalla, Plomin, Defries, and Fulker (as cited in Brody,
1992, p. 257) using behavioral genetic analysis to determine the relationship between children¡¯s
IQ and reading achievement indicated that reading achievement and IQ were equally heritable and
¡°estimated that 90% of the covariance between reading achievement and IQ was attributed to
shared genetic covariance¡± (Brody, 1992, p. 257). It is highly doubtful that NCLB will alter
children¡¯s reading achievement to any appreciable and enduring extent and the consequences for
not doing so under this Draconian legislation are grim for not only the professional administration
and teaching staff, but for all of those involved in public education including the students the law
was intentionally designed to help.
Inappropriate Allegiance to the Medical Model
In addition to ill-conceived legislation, the bewildering breath of theories and practices support
the assumption that there is little or no unifying knowledge either in educational practice or
the field of school psychology. In both, there is an absence of any substantial cumulative knowledge
or consensus on practices. Although these assumptions may be contested, they are supported
by the observation that in the 15,000 or so school districts throughout the United States there is no
convincing evidence that any one of these, through their varied and independent curricula, policies,
or practices, produce academic achievement beyond that which would be predicted years
before on the basis of indices of cognitive functioning. ¡°That is to say, children who are intellectually
average, evidence average academic achievement, no matter where they go to school. This
578 Kehle and Bray
inescapable observation is particularly apparent in the later grades.¡± (Kehle, Clark, & Jenson,
1993, p. 158). ¡°Jencks concluded variation among schools in the U.S. might account for as little as
2%¨C3% of the variance in academic achievement after the composition of the social class background
of the school were taken into account¡± (Brody, 1992, p. 260). A critical question is that
given the countless variation in how teachers actually teach, why, after controlling for cognitive
variables, is there no evidence of variation in achievement? ¡°Variations in what children learn in
school are not attributable to variations in the nature of the schools but are attributable to the
characteristics that children bring to the schools.¡± (Brody, 1992, p. 260). Further, it can be posited
that children¡¯s psychological health has not been appreciably promoted through the unique practices
of school psychologists or other health service providers in any particular school district. It
is highly probable that if data were aggregated even at the school building level, similar results
would be observed. That is to say, when cognitive variables are controlled across the 120,000
public and private schools, no one school would be more effective than any other in promoting
children¡¯s academic competence.
In summary, there exists no convincing empirical evidence that public education is more
effective today than it was at the beginning of the 20th century (Witt, 1986). Nor is there any
convincing evidence that students are more psychologically healthy or exhibit more civility than
they did 100 years ago. Nevertheless, children do learn. However, it may have little or nothing to
do with the innumerable methodologies employed in the schools. For all the numerous and detailed
types of instructional interventions to result in effectively equal academic achievements may
suggest a common foundation of the learning process. Applying Glass¡¯s (2001) statement to educational
practice, the implausibility that the overwhelming variety of specific educational strategies
and interventions, curricula and practices, delivered with in a bewildering multitude of
district and school-level policies would yield indistinguishable academic outcomes is a strong clue
that either it is instead a set of often unacknowledged common elements that is effective in promoting
student learning, or else it is a set of processes residing largely in the students and simply
mobilized by being in school that promotes educational obtainments.
Similarly,Wampold¡¯s (2001) argument to replace the medical model with a contextual model
of practice has implications for both school psychology and education in general. The medical
model has its origins in psychotherapy and behaviorism and includes the identification of a ¡°disorder
(hysteria), the explanation for the disorder (repressed traumatic events), a mechanism of
change (insight in unconscious), and specific therapeutic actions (free association)¡± (Wampold,
2001, p. 11). ¡°Specificity, the critical aspect of the medical model, implies that the specific therapeutic
ingredients are remedial for the disorder. The specific ingredients are assumed to be responsible
(i.e., necessary) for client change or progress toward therapeutic goals¡± (Wampold, 2001,
p. 14).
The medical model is commonly employed in school psychological practice and is synonymous
with the diagnostic prescriptive approach. There is identification of a problem through
observation and diagnostic testing, an explanation for the problem and rationale for change that is
in concert with the school psychologist¡¯s theoretical orientation, and implementation of an intervention
or treatment that contains specific components that are assumed to be efficacious.
According toWampold (2001), the medical model encompasses most, if not all, behaviorally
based interventions. Consequently, the empirically supported treatment movement is also firmly
based on the medical model in that it is predominated by behavioral and cognitive-behavioral
treatments (Wampold, 2001).
The medical model is enormously popular in school psychology in that it is literally required
as the basis of psycho-educational evaluations, and is the foundation of most university training
programs. Further, the NCLB legislation, in that it is based on the medical model, will engender
Reducing the Gap Between Research and Practice 579
considerable pressure on school psychologists to continue their allegiance to the model. However,
there is an alternative to the medical model which Wampold labeled the ¡°contextual¡± model that
emphasizes a holistic common factors approach. The model is termed contextual because it emphasizes
that treatment effectiveness is due primarily to common factors. Although there certainly are
common elements across all treatments, the contextual model assumes that these common elements
cannot be isolated or investigated independently.
The contextual model is supported by the observation ¡°that all of the many specific types of
psychotherapeutic treatments achieve virtually equal¡ªor insignificantly different¡ªbenefits because
of a common core of curative processes¡± (Glass, 2001, p. ix). ¡°The implausibility that the great
variety of specific ingredients in the multitude of psychotherapeutic approaches would yield indistinguishable
outcomes is a strong clue that either it is instead a set of often unacknowledged
common elements that is effective, or else it is a set of processes residing largely in the clients and
merely mobilized by therapy that carries the power to improve clients¡¯ lives¡± (Glass, 2001, p. ix).
Basically, Wampold supports the findings that psychotherapy is beneficial. However, the
particular type of therapy employed does not account for any appreciable amount of the variance
in the patient¡¯s positive outcome. What is important are nonspecific effects of common elements
across different therapeutic approaches, including the relationship formed between therapist and
patient.Wampold argued against the notion that detailed treatment protocols are beneficial, instead
he recommended that careful monitoring of the individual¡¯s progress be conducted. Such an approach
is inherent in single case research.
Wampold summarized the differences between the medical and contextual models through
differences in their respective differential hypotheses and evidentiary rules. With respect to differential
hypotheses, the effects of treatment in comparison to no treatment, both the medical and
the contextual models would predict efficacious outcomes; however the relative efficacy of each
model would differ in that the medical model would predict variation in treatment efficacy, and the
contextual model would predict uniform efficacy. The medical model would predict that positive
outcomes would be a result of specific components of the therapy, whereas the contextual model
would not. However, both models would predict that the treatment would be superior to a placebo,
and the placebo would be superior to no treatment. Also supporting the contextual model, there is
no evidence for specificity.AsWampold stated, there are countless examples of treatments that are
clearly positive and result in the client¡¯s betterment, however. ¡°the psychological explanations for
the benefits have failed to be verified¡± (p. 147). Even the components of the popular and highly
respected cognitive-behavioral treatment, ¡°are apparently not responsible for the benefits of this
treatment¡± (p. 148). Rather than any specific component causing positive psychotherapeutic outcomes,
it is the single factor of the working alliance that is the key component to producing
positive outcomes. ¡°It appears that the relationship accounts for dramatically more of the variance
in outcomes than the totality of specific ingredients¡± (p. 158). Additional strong support for the
contextual model was that the effects of adherence to treatment protocols were negligible. Finally,
with respect to Wampold¡¯s evidentiary rules supporting the contextual model, the research on
therapist effects would indicate that it is not the specific treatment, nor adherence to a specific
treatment protocol that effects positive change, but that the person of the therapist. The medical
model would predict that the therapist¡¯s technical expertise would account for variability of outcome.
¡°The evidence is clear that the type of treatment is irrelevant, and adherence to a protocol
is misguided, but yet the therapist, within each of the treatments, makes a tremendous difference¡±
(p. 202).
Perhaps, there is also a common elements explanation of those noticed positive gains from
school psychological practices. It may be that it is not necessarily the theoretical orientation, or
particular school psychological intervention, but nonspecific effects, common elements inherent
580 Kehle and Bray
across the varied and numerous strategies and interventions that are important in promoting children
academic and social competencies.
To help close the gap between science and practice, one would think it would be beneficial to
closely examine and compare the effectiveness of practices based on the medical, as opposed to
the contextual model. However, according to Wampold (2001), this comparison is not possible
because one cannot construct a manualized contextual model treatment. In another sense, all
treatments are examples of the contextual model. So, when one compares cognitivebehavioral
treatment for depression with an interpersonal treatment for depression, one is
also comparing a cognitive-behavioral model with a contextual model. If the two treatments
are equally effective, it is because of their respective specific ingredients or because both are
instances of contextual model treatments? (p. 27)
Another explanation for the existence of the gap is the lack of clear definitions and consensus
of what are the dependent variables. Although there is a continual plea for evidenced-based interventions
that affect measurable outcomes, there is no consensus on how these outcomes are related
to the definition of an educated and psychologically healthy individual. Specifically, a problem
with educational, and particularly school psychological practice, is that there is no clear definition
of either an educated or psychologically healthy individual. This is most probably why, unlike the
hard sciences, there has been no noticeable incremental improvement in either educational or
school psychological practice. Unlike many examples noted in the sciences, psychology and education,
because of the lack of accumulated knowledge, have not ever experienced a ¡°breakthrough.¡±
The acid test is to be able to convincingly argue that children are better educated and
psychologically healthier in contemporary society in contrast to their counterparts in some earlier
period in history.
A Unifying Theory of Education
Barclay¡¯s (1991) quotation of Henri Poincare is the microcosm of the plight of school psychology,
and for that matter education. ¡°Science is built up of facts, as a house is built up of stones,
but an accumulation of facts is no more a science than a heap of stones is a house¡± (p. 21).Without
a unifying theory, or even a clear definition of what the dependent variables should be, it is
practically impossible to realize an accumulation of knowledge that promotes incremental improvement
in both school psychological, and educational practice. To seriously address ways of closing
the gap between research and practice in school psychology there has to be theoretical development
that addresses the definition of the proposed end product of the educational experience.
We suggest that the Resources, Intimacy, Competence, and Health theory (R.I.C.H.; Kehle,
1989; Kehle, 1999; Kehle & Bray, 2004; Kehle et al., 1993) presents a tenable unifying theory of
education that would most probably provide sufficient focus to allow for cumulative knowledge in
school psychological and educational research. The R.I.C.H. theory incorporates aspects of Darwin¡¯s
(1859) On the Origin of Species, Russell¡¯s (1930) Conquest of Happiness, Wilson¡¯s Consilience
(1998), and Skinner¡¯s (1971) Beyond Freedom and Dignity.
The R.I.C.H. theory defines psychological health as being synonymous with an educated and
happy individual. ¡°With respect to the R.I.C.H. theory, these individuals have four characteristics
including resources, intimacy, competence, and health. The four characteristics are interrelated to
the extent that they incorporate each other in their definitions, encompass all possible reinforcers,
are relatively obtainable by all individuals, and the improvement in any one, results in improvement
of the remaining three¡± (Kehle & Bray, 2004, p. 43). These four characteristics essentially
encapsulate Bertrand Russell¡¯s (1930) definition of happiness.
Reducing the Gap Between Research and Practice 581
Resources. The appropriate allocation of resources results in a feeling of independence or
professionalism which is defined as being synonymous with a sense of individual freedom, or a
sense of control over one¡¯s time and daily life. The assessment of the appropriate allocation of
resources is based on the degree the individual can initiate and maintain friendships, establish
competence, and enjoy physical health.
Intimacy. Intimacy is defined as friendship. It involves empathy and the appreciation and
enjoyment of a friend¡¯s company. To initiate and maintain an intimate relationship requires an
allocation of resources, competence, and physical health.
Competence. The sense of competence is the consequence of being competent relative to
some standard. Competence is attributed to one¡¯s own abilities. The feeling of competence, in
addition to being the consequence of some competent behavior, is also specific. One¡¯s sense of
competence is the consequence of having resources, enjoying intimacy, and physical health.
Health. In a relative sense, the individual is aware of practices that are conducive to physical
health and also has an allegiance to these practices. A physically healthy individual is defined
as one who is independent, enjoys intimacy, and feels competent. (Kehle & Bray, 2004).
Again, the characteristics of resources, intimacy, competence, and health at first inspection
would not appear to be related. However, as indicated above they are so highly interrelated they
essentially can be considered synonymous with each other. As previously stated, if one of the
characteristics is promoted, so is the remaining three. If one is diminished, so is the remaining
three. Also, as stated above, each of the four characteristics is defined by the remaining three. For
example the definition of a physically healthy person is one who is independent, enjoys intimacy,
and feels competent.
Perhaps applying Glass¡¯s (2001) statements to school psychological and educational practice
and in concert with the contextual model, that positive change may be primarily the consequence
of a set of often unacknowledged common elements. Or else it is a set of processes residing largely
in the students and that are merely mobilized by intentionally or unintentionally promoting the
R.I.C.H. characteristics that carry the power to improve student¡¯s lives. For example, promoting
student independence by allowing them to largely design their own curriculum would also promote
the remaining R.I.C.H. characteristics (Kehle & Bray, 2004). Employing symbolic or material
rewards may be interpreted by the student as manipulation and control, which is inversely
related to the student¡¯s sense of independence, and most probably would function to insidiously
erode the student¡¯s intrinsic motivation to learn (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 2001) can consequently
diminish the remaining R.I.C.H. characteristics. When rewards are used to promote the student¡¯s
sense of independence, his or her intrinsic motivation to pursue learning is apparently enhanced.
Deci and colleagues¡¯ meta-analytic study on the effects of extrinsic and intrinsic rewards, indicated
that choice is important in promoting student independence and therefore intrinsic motivation.
Further, the R.I.C.H. would predict the student¡¯s sense of competence, intimacy, and physical
health would also improve.
According to Kehle and Bray (2004),
the student should have a sense of ¡°not working¡± in that he or she intrinsically enjoys their
selections of school environments and choices of what they learn. It would result in a lack of
bifurcation between the student¡¯s private and public lives. For example, a student assigned
homework that is perceived as nothing other than ¡°busy¡± work would definitely have a sense
of working. It is perceived as an unnecessary, and educationally illegitimate intrusion on his
or her time. External contingencies that are perceived as manipulative, are inversely related
to a sense of the individual student¡¯s sense of developing professionalism. In accordance with
582 Kehle and Bray
the R.I.C.H. theory, the student learns best that which he or she is intrinsically interested.
(p. 45)
As Kehle and Bray (2004) indicated, the R.I.C.H. characteristics subsume all possible human
reinforcers. It is difficult to add another characteristic, these four sufficiently provide a definition
of an educated, happy, and psychologically healthy individual. An interesting observation of the
numerous definitions and theories of psychopathology is that they all include a component of
anxiety or fear. The R.I.C.H. characteristics define life relatively free from fear.
Education, at its heart, is elitist based on the inescapable observation that smart kids do better
at it. No matter what is done in an attempt to improve the not so smart kids¡¯ academic performance,
the smart kids will do better. However, in accord with the R.I.C.H. theory, all children can
acquire the four characteristics; all children can be learn to be educated as defined by the theory.
According to Kehle and Bray (2004)
In a relative sense, all children are fully capable of learning to promote their independence,
establishing and maintaining intimacy, experiencing the satisfaction of competence, and enjoying
physical health. In addition, the four characteristics allow for the evaluation of processes.
Interventions can be evaluated relative to their worth in promoting movement toward the
attainment of the four R.I.C.H. characteristics. If the educational system requires the preschooler
to become skilled at walking a balance beam, its worth, or lack of it, can be judged
relative to its influence in promoting the R.I.C.H. characteristics. (p. 45)
The R.I.C.H theory would predict that when mothers, regardless of their cultural background,
are asked the question what do they want their child to have, or become, in adulthood, they would
respond in accordance with the R.I.C.H. theory. They want their children to be happy, that is to
have resources, intimacy, competence, and health (Kehle, 1999). Although, the R.I.C.H. characteristics
are what most parents want for their children, the majority of them nevertheless assume
that to acquire these one must have high educational attainment, high family income, marital
status, and religious commitment. However, as Lykken and Tellegen (1996) stated, less than 3% of
the variance in happiness or subjective well-being can be attributed to these misguided but highly
popular and cherished goals.
In summary, we have attempted to present an argument that the existence of the gap between
science and practice in school psychology is the result of an overallegiance to the medical model
and the lack of a clear definition of an educated and psychologically healthy student. An additional
problem for school psychological practice is that it is entrenched in, and highly influenced by,
general education that has a history of continual reform and negligible evidence of an accumulated
knowledge base that has resulted in demonstratively improving the education of children. Perhaps,
because of this universal observation regarding the lack of effectiveness in educational practice,
there have numerous and also almost continuous efforts to legislate reform. Most of these have
met with failure as NCLB most probably will.
The contextual model assumes that the positive effects of school psychological practice and
education may reside in the persons of the school psychologist and the classroom teacher. The
R.I.C.H. theory assumes that all children have the capacity, in a relative sense, to acquire the
theory¡¯s characteristics. It was also argued that perhaps the goal of education and school psychology
should be consistent with what parents want for their children. That is they wish that their
children possessed resources to the extent they are relatively free and independent, have and
maintain intimate friendships, enjoy a feeling of competence, and benefit from physical health.
In summary, we presented arguments that the gap between science and practice in school
psychology may be perpetuated by an overallegiance to the medical model, and exacerbated by the
lack of a clear definition of an educated and psychologically healthy student. Further, general
education has historically lacked evidence of an accumulating knowledge base that would result in
Reducing the Gap Between Research and Practice 583
a continuing and incremental improvement of the educational process. Perhaps this lack of demonstrable
improvement of educational practice is largely the motivation for continual reform efforts
and legal mandates that are often equally ineffective and at times even detrimental such as the
NCLB act. To practice evidenced-based school psychology within this environment is not only
challenging, but often frustrating and threatening to one¡¯s professional moral and sense of worth.
584 Kehle and Bray
 
 
copyright [c] ©2004 ipcf All right reserved
Rms.,1102-03,11/F.,Kowloon Bldg., 555 Nathan Road, Mongkok, Kowloon, Hong Kong.